C O M M E N T A R Y
Open Access
In memoriam: John Lisman
–
commentaries
on CaMKII as a memory molecule
Mark F. Bear
1*
, Sam F. Cooke
2*
, Karl Peter Giese
2*
, Bong-Kiun Kaang
3*
, Mary B. Kennedy
4*
, Ji-il Kim
3
,
Richard G. M. Morris
5*
and Pojeong Park
3
Abstract
Shortly before he died in October 2017, John Lisman submitted an invited review to Molecular Brain on
‘
Criteria for
identifying the molecular basis of the engram (CaMKII, PKM
ζ
)
’
. John had no opportunity to read the referees
’
comments,
and as a mark of the regard in which he was held by the neuroscience community the Editors decided to publish his
review as submitted. This obituary takes the
form of a series of commentaries on Lisman
’
sreview.Atthesametimewe
are publishing as a separate article a longer response by Todd Sacktor and André Fenton entitled
‘
What does LTP tell us
about the roles of CaMKII and PKM
ζ
in memory?
’
which presents the case for a rival memory molecule, PKM
ζ
.
John Lisman 1944
–
2017
Contributed by Karl Peter Giese
Last October John Lisman sadly passed at the age of
73. John was an exceptional neuroscientist who made a
wide variety of seminal contributions. His very highly
cited work includes the development of novel theories
about the role of bursts in information processing [
1],
the storage of short-term memories in oscillatory cycles
[
2], the function of the hippocampal-VTA in long-term
memory formation [
3], and the CaMKII (Ca
2+
/calmodu-
lin-dependent protein kinase II) hypothesis for memory
storage. The latter CaMKII hypothesis was John
’
s most
important contribution. It originated from a theoretical
paper in 1985 where he proposed that the molecular
basis for memory storage could be an autophosphorylat-
ing kinase that persistently maintains activity at synapses
[
4]. Shortly after John
’
s publication CaMKII was
biochemically purified from synapses and shown to have
an autophosphorylation switch. Together, with John
’
s
theoretical paper this led to the CaMKII hypothesis for
memory storage [
5]. The attractiveness of this hypothesis
motivated many eminent neuroscientists to study the
function of CaMKII and its isoforms in learning and
memory. Accumulating experimental evidences not only
supported, but also provided some problems for the
CaMKII hypothesis, which motivated refinements that
to keep the hypothesis viable [
6
–
9]. Last year John was
very delighted to publish experimental proof for his
CaMKII hypothesis after more than 30 years of research
[
9, 10]. Next to his pioneering work on memory storage,
John was also a true scholar who was open for discus-
sion of other viewpoints. I experienced this myself when
questioning the CaMKII hypothesis over many years.
John will be remembered not only for his seminal con-
tributions, but also for the breadth and generosity of his
academic spirit in advancing our understanding of the
brain.
The enigma of memory maintenance: Commentary
on Criteria for identifying the molecular basis of the
engram (CaMKII, PKM
ζ
)
Contributed by Sam F. Cooke and Mark F. Bear
The late John Lisman was one of the great thinkers of
neuroscience. He posed some of the biggest questions and
provided eminently testable hypotheses. Amongst his
many contributions, he is probably best known for illus-
trating how the enzyme Calcium/calmodulin-dependent
* Correspondence:
mbear@mit.edu
; samuel.cooke@kcl.ac.uk
;
karl.giese@kcl.ac.uk
; kaang@snu.ac.kr
; kennedym@caltech.edu
;
r.g.m.morris@ed.ac.uk
1
Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Department of Brain and
Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA
02139, USA
2
King
’
s College London, Department of Basic and Clinical Neuroscience,
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park,
London SE5 8AF, UK
3
Department of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-gu,
Seoul, Republic of Korea
4
The Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of
Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
5
Laboratory for Cognitive Neuroscience, Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences,
Edinburgh Neuroscience, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK
© The Author(s). 2018
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to
the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver
(
http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/
) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-018-0419-y
protein kinase II (CaMKII) may serve as a molecular
switch to maintain changes in synaptic strength and
long-term memory [
4, 5, 11, 12].
In the early 1980
’
s, variations on Hebbian synaptic
plasticity had started to predominate as the favoured
theoretical mechanism by which learning occurs in the
brain, several decades after the key concepts had first
been formalized by Donald Hebb [
13] and a decade after
the first example of lasting Hebbian plasticity, known as
long-term potentiation (LTP), was observed in the brain
of living organisms [
14, 15]. LTP has several remarkable
properties: It is
“
input specific
”
, meaning that it can
occur selectively at a very small fraction of the synapses
formed on a given neuron [
16]; it is rapidly induced
within seconds [
15]; and it can last months in vivo [
17]
and many hours in brain slices [
18]. These properties
are ideal for encoding episodic memories, but present
challenges for understanding the underlying cellular
biology. Longevity of LTP could be explained by stable
activation of gene expression and new protein synthesis,
but how would these new proteins be continuously sup-
plied to (or captured by) only the potentiated synapses,
and how would this mechanism account for the rapid
induction of LTP at synapses far away from the nucleus?
On the other hand, rapid induction and synapse specifi-
city could be readily accounted for by local
post-translational modification of pre-existing synaptic
proteins, but how would this be maintained in the face
of protein turnover?
Much thought has been expended on these questions
and the most influential solutions came from Lisman [
4]
and, independently, from another deep thinker, Francis
Crick [
19]. To address these twin problems, the concept of
a
‘
memorase
’
was introduced, which is an enzyme com-
posed of multiple subunits t
hat can auto-regulate each
other to sustain activity and maintain synaptic change by
continually modifying key syn
aptic proteins. Thus, newly
synthesized subunits could be incorporated at any time to
replace outgoing subunits and switched into an active form
through post-translational m
odification by a neighbouring
subunit, thereby mai
ntaining the overall
‘
active
’
state of the
molecular complex. Lisman and Goldring went so far as to
posit the likeliest candidate, CaMKII [
5, 11], which is a kin-
ase that typically exists as a 12-subunit, double hexamer
structure that autophosphorylates neighbouring subunits to
maintain those subunits in an open and active state [
20].
Among many other actions, active CaMKII phosphorylates
post-synaptic AMPA receptors to enhance their efficacy,
which is a key expression mechanism for LTP and memory
[
21] and produces other syn
aptic alterations [
22]. This con-
cept was highly influential because it addressed both prob-
lems: (1) LTP could initially be induced by activation of
existing, local CaMKII doub
le hexamers by calcium influx
through the NMDA receptor and (2) new,
unphosphorylated subunits
couldbeincorporatedinto
CaMKII double hexamers without interrupting its activity
state and, thanks to autophosphorylation of neighbouring
subunits, perpetuate its activity state in a way that is im-
mune to molecular turnover and local to the synapse at
which potentiation originally occurred [
23
–
25].
Even the subsequent discoveries that hippocampal
synapses were bidirectionally modifiable, allowing low
frequency (1 Hz) stimulation to induce calcium-
dependent, Hebbian long-term depression (LTD) [
26,
27
], and that this LTD was mediated in part by protein
phosphatases [
28
–
30], was presaged by Lisman in a
modified CaMKII model [
12]. In this updated model,
phosphatases such as calcineurin respond to lower cal-
cium concentrations than CaMKII and act to dephos-
phorylate CaMKII targets and CaMKII itself, thereby
setting up a bistable switch at synapses that can adopt a
potentiated or a depressed state. Through its elegance
and simplicity, this overall model has been highly influ-
ential, and it became the textbook example of how last-
ing synaptic modification may occur at individual
synapses. Much experimental evidence supported the
model as well, notably the demonstration that phosphor-
ylation of the T286 residue of
α
CaMKII, which main-
tains CaMKII in an open state and is the key site for
sustained activity through autophosphorylation, is re-
quired for LTP and memory [
31]. However, two major
experimental observations were strongly at odds with
the model: First, blockade of CaMKII activity with se-
lective inhibitors prevents LTP induction and learning,
but does not prevent LTP maintenance or memory if ap-
plied very shortly after induction/learning [
32
–
34] and,
second, CaMKII activity is not sustained for more than a
few minutes after LTP induction [
35
–
37].
While CaMKII has failed to be fully anointed as the
molecular basis of the engram, PKM
ζ
, which is an atyp-
ical isoform of the calcium-dependent protein kinase
(PKC), has emerged as an alternative candidate to sup-
port long-lasting, synapse-specific potentiation and
memory. The model of how PKM
ζ
may mediate
synapse-specific potentiation and remain immune to
molecular turnover is somewhat different from CaMKII,
but no less elegant [
38]. In this case, mRNA transcripts
encoding PKM
ζ
(transcribed from the PRKC
ζ
gene), are
freely available within the cell, but translation into PKM
ζ
protein is blocked by a protein called PIN1 (protein
interacting with NIMA1) [
39]. Only once the LTP/learn-
ing event activates calcium-dependent synaptic signal-
ling, which includes CaMKII, is this PIN1 block on the
translation of PKM
ζ
removed [
40]. PKM
ζ
can then act
to increase the number of post-synaptic AMPA recep-
tors through interactions with NSF-dependent AMPA
receptor trafficking [
41, 42]. Critically, the newly-
synthesized PKM
ζ
then suppresses further PIN1 activity,
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 2 of 12
perpetuating its own synthesis [
39]. For this model to
work as a synapse-specific potentiation system, it is as-
sumed that protein synthesis is localised to each synapse
due to proximal positioning of ribosome complexes, for
which there is substantial evidence [
43, 44].
A wealth of evidence is consistent with this kinase
serving as a molecular basis of the engram. Most strik-
ingly, application of an inhibitor peptide, ZIP, which was
designed to be highly selective for PKM
ζ
, reverses
long-established LTP and memory [
45, 46]. Additionally,
the synthesis of constitutively active PKM
ζ
is increased
for at least a month after LTP is induced [
47], in con-
trast to observations of CaMKII activity [
35, 36]. These
findings have had a dramatic impact on the field of
learning and memory because, until very recently, there
had not been an example of a molecule that could be
targeted to erase established LTP and memory. As with
CaMKII, however, major experimental counter-evidence
clouds the waters. First, genetic knockouts of the PRKC
ζ
gene (encoding PKC
ζ
and PKM
ζ
) do not prevent LTP or
learning and memory in a mouse [
48, 49] and, second,
the specific inhibitor ZIP is equally effective at erasing
LTP and memory in these knockout mice [
49, 50], dem-
onstrating that it is not as specific as it was designed to
be. Thus, both CaMKII and PKM
ζ
are compelling at a
conceptual level, but their supporters are left with some
major explaining to do.
In his article
‘
Criteria for Identifying the Molecular
Basis of the Engram
’
, John Lisman has built a case for
CaMKII as the strongest candidate for the molecular
basis of the engram [
10]. He does so by comparing it
directly to PKM
ζ
and asking how well the experimental
evidence for each stack up in relation to key criteria:
First, that the enzyme is necessary for LTP/memory.
Second, that its overactivity will saturate synaptic
strength and occlude LTP/memory and, third, that spe-
cific blockade after induction/learning will lead to LTP/
memory erasure. He also discusses a fourth consider-
ation, which is how well each mechanism could serve as
a local maintenance system. While Lisman has made a
compelling case, it is important to be clear that the evi-
dence has been selectively presented. First, Lisman ar-
gues that there is more evidence for CaMKII being
necessary for LTP/memory than there is for PKM
ζ
,
based on the observation that LTP and memory are
retained in PKC
ζ
knockout mice [
48, 49], while the
T286A
α
CaMKII point mutant mice, that do not main-
tain autonomous activity of
α
CaMKII through autophos-
phorylation, show very little LTP and highly deficient
hippocampus-dependent memory [
31]. However, this is
a somewhat unfair comparison. Lisman neglects to dis-
cuss the fact that the full
α
CaMKII knockout mice,
which provides the most direct comparison with the
PKM
ζ
knockout mouse, retain a substantial amount of
LTP at these synapses [
51, 52] and show slowed
hippocampus-dependent learning that can nonetheless
reach a normal asymptote over time [
53], likely due to
compensation by the beta isoform of CaMKII [
54]. This
type of genetic compensation is precisely the explanation
that seems most likely for the maintained plasticity and
memory in the PKC
ζ
knockout mouse, where the iota/
lamda isoform of PKC likely supports most duties in the
absence of PKM
ζ
[50]. Furthermore, even where LTP ap-
pears completely absent at the Schaffer collateral-CA1
synapses in the T286A point mutant, LTP can be in-
duced by alternative NMDA receptor-dependent means
at medial perforant path-granule cell synapses in the
dentate gyrus subfield of the hippocampus [
55], and last-
ing memory can be attained with overtraining [
56]. In
the strongest sense of
‘
necessity
’
for LTP/memory main-
tenance, therefore, neither CaMKII nor PKM
ζ
meets the
criterion. However, there is compelling evidence that,
under normal circumstances, both play a critical role in
many forms of synaptic plasticity and memory.
A more complex criterion to address is the stipulation
that overactivity of a memory mechanism should satur-
ate synaptic strength and thereby occlude LTP/memory.
As presented by Lisman in
‘
Criteria for Identifying the
Molecular Basis of the Engram
’
, overactivity of CaMKII
increases synaptic strength and prevents LTP induction
or learning [
9, 57], while a similar treatment for PKM
ζ
enhances LTP and learning [
58]. Thus, CaMKII appears
to meet the occlusion criterion while PKM
ζ
fails. How-
ever, if we were to select a different set of representative
studies, then overexpression of activated CaMKII has
been shown to enhance some forms of LTP [
59] while
overexpression of activated PKM
ζ
increases synaptic
transmission and blocks further LTP [
60]. Thus, there is
little consensus yet as to which of these two molecules
best meets the occlusion criterion, reflecting the fact
that, despite much excellent work, the true molecular
basis of the engram is not fully established.
Next, Lisman argues that while ZIP is highly effective
at erasing LTP and memory, his laboratory has identified
better ways of doing precisely the same for CaMKII.
They find in slices that a dose (20
μ
M) of the
cell-permeable peptide tatCN21 that both inhibits CaM-
KII and interferes with the association of the enzyme
with postsynaptic NMDA receptors is sufficient to de-
press basal synaptic transmission and erase established
LTP [
61]. Additionally, a transiently-expressing herpes
simplex viral (HSV) vector, which temporarily produces
a dominant negative form of CaMKII, erases established
place avoidance memory [
9]. Moreover, he states that an
additional important test is included in these experi-
ments that has not been conducted in the key ZIP stud-
ies [
45
, 46, 60], namely that synapses are re-potentiated,
and animals re-learn after erasure, demonstrating that
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 3 of 12
the necessary synaptic machinery remained intact des-
pite the presence of a foreign agent. This is a crucial
point, and more effort should certainly be made to
evaluate the erasing effects of ZIP, given some indication
that ZIP may damage cells [
62] or target other neural
processes than synaptic function [
63]. However, it is
worth noting that evidence exists that plasticity can be
restored after erasure by ZIP [
64] and that alternative
pharmaceutical inhibitors of PKM catalytic activity also
erase established LTP [
60].
The findings with a high dose of the inhibitor tatCN21
stand in contrast to the effects of a lower dose. Unlike
other pharmaceutical inhibitors such as KN62 and KN93,
tatCN21 at a dose of 5
μ
M inhibits CaMKII phosphoryl-
ation of substrates even when the enzyme has been
switched into an activated, autonomous state [
33, 61].
Interestingly however, at this dose LTP maintenance is
unaffected. This finding therefore calls into question the
original model of CaMKII as a self-perpetuating
‘
memor-
ase
’
[33]. To reconcile these findings, it is necessary to
further postulate existence of a critical contribution of
the physical association of CaMKII and NMDA receptors
in the postsynaptic density. At high doses, tatCN21 and
related peptides disrupt CaMKII binding to the NMDAR
[
61, 65], and it is argued that the dose dependency of the
tatCN21 peptide on LTP maintenance is reflective of the
need for a CaMKII-NMDAR complex in LTP mainten-
ance. Sanhueza and Lisman have suggested that this
interaction can contribute to the structural modifications
that are believed to support long-term functional modifi-
cations of synapses [
65], and more work is required to
understand what these structural changes may be. While
this new model revives a critical role for CaMKII in
long-term synaptic modifications underlying memory, it
is somewhat removed from the elegance of the original
Crick/Lisman hypotheses [
4, 5, 11, 19].
A last point for discussion is Lisman
’
scontentionthatit
is difficult to justify PKM
ζ
as a synapse-specific mechanism
of maintenance given the fact that that it appears to be in-
creased in expression throughout neurons after learning
and has a major effect on gene expression at the nucleus as
aresult[
66]. Bearing in mind that PKM
ζ
canonlybeinan
active state, this contrasts dramatically with the observation
that CaMKII activity remains very local to the spine where
it is induced [
36, 37], and suggests that if PKM
ζ
were the
primary molecular basis of the engram it would likely exert
its influence on surr
ounding synapses, a prospect that is
clearly at odds with the input specificity of LTP [
16]and
Hebbian synaptic plasticity in general [
13]. Lisman certainly
raises a valid concern here, but it is also perfectly possible
that PKM
ζ
does not reach sufficient concentration in
neighbouring spines to suppress PIN1 activity and switch
the synapse into a stable
‘
memory
’
state. This is an issue
that requires serious exper
imental investigation. The
proposal from Lisman that PKM
ζ
is, for this reason, better
suited as a heterosynaptic scaling mechanism does, how-
ever, seem flawed, as scaling is proposed to be a homeo-
static mechanism that potenti
ates synapses after prolonged
periods of reduced neural activity [
67, 68], whereas it is pro-
nounced, event-related stimul
ation of neural activity that
leads to PKM
ζ
expression [
47, 69, 70]. It would be very use-
ful to image PKM
ζ
distribution and concentration as LTP is
induced at single synapses, as this would provide some
insight into its differential contributions to synapse-specific
and whole-cell memory mechanisms.
John Lisman made an extraordinary contribution to
neuroscience. He strived to pull together threads of data
from multiple sources to clear
ly articulate models for mem-
ory storage, lay out the critical tests of these models, and
challenge the field (includin
g his own laboratory) to refute
them with rigorous experiments. His voice will be sorely
missed in this debate. After much work from many scien-
tists we cannot question the idea that CaMKII autophos-
phorylation is required for learning and LTP induction,
proving that the
‘
memorase
’
property of the kinase does at
least bridge a critical period between NMDA receptor
opening and AMPA receptor modification. CaMKII may
well play a key role in memory maintenance, although
through a different mechanism
than originally envisioned,
and these are just two parts of a much larger scientific leg-
acy. For science to progress there must always be innovative
ideas, but there must always be rigorous testing and criti-
cism of these ideas to determine their validity. John Lisman
has been a powerful force in both processes, as illustrated
in polemical pieces such as
‘
Criteria for identifying the Mo-
lecular Basis of the Engram
’
. His article succeeds in illus-
trating that the evidence for CaMKII as a memory
maintenance mechanism is no weaker than that for PKM
ζ
.
The clearest message that we can take from this is that
much more work needs to be done to understand the mo-
lecular basis of the engram. Whether there is truly one
mechanism that governs memory remains open to ques-
tion. Typically, evolution would not arrive at such a fragile
solution, and tends to re-purpose a multitude of existing
mechanisms to fulfil similar, overlapping roles [
71]. Both
PKM
ζ
and CaMKII are conserved systems, being present
and operational in synaptic plasticity in both invertebrates
and vertebrates [
72, 73], but there are many such conserved
candidate systems. It therefor
e seems unlikely that either
CaMKII or PKM
ζ
serves as the singular molecular basis of
the engram [
74], but there is clear and compelling evidence
that each plays a central role.
Commentary on
‘
Criteria for identifying the mo-
lecular basis of the engram (CaMKII, PKM
ζ
)
’
.
Contributed by Karl Peter Giese.
How the brain stores memory is a fundamental ques-
tion in neuroscience. John Lisman pioneered the mo-
lecular investigation of this question. Back in 1985 he
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 4 of 12
hypothesized that autophosphorylation of a kinase may
be a molecular switch that leads to persistent, memory-
storing signaling at synapses [4; for independent pio-
neering work see, 19]. Over the last three decades John
Lisman
’
s theoretical work inspired many experiments
that eventually gave rise to a very sophisticated CaMKII
hypothesis of memory storage [
5
–
7, 10]. Later, a differ-
ent hypothesis emerged, stating that PKM
ζ
could pro-
vide memory-storing signaling at synapses [
38].
Currently, it is unclear whether the CaMKII hypothesis
and the PKM
ζ
hypothesis are mutually exclusive, or
whether both kinases together mediate memory storage
(see also, [
74]). In his last review John Lisman critically
discusses the evidence for and against the two hypoth-
eses [
10]. He also explains fundamental concepts and in-
troduces criteria for the molecular basis of memory
storage. Whilst I think that this review is very important,
I disagree with some points, as follows.
One of the proposed criteria is the saturation/occlusion
test. The idea behind this test is that overexpression of an
activated form of the memory-storing protein after mem-
ory formation is completed should increase most synaptic
weights, preventing memory access [
9]. However, this test
is confounded if the protein of interest is involved in
retrieval-induced memory destabilization that precedes re-
consolidation/restabilization [
75]. Specifically, this is the
case for CaMKII. Using inducible overactivation of CaM-
KII, Joe Tsien
’
s lab showed that increased CaMKII at the
time of retrieval causes memory loss [
76]. Further, they
established that this is not due to deficits in accessing the
acquired memory, because switching off CaMKII overacti-
vation did not restore the lost memory. Therefore, it was
concluded that CaMKII overactivation enhances memory
destabilization, leading to memory erasure, and that this
process requires protein degradation [
77]. Consistent with
this idea, CaMKII regulates protein degradation at the
synapse [
78] and blocking CaMKII activity impairs
retrieval-induced protein degradation and memory
destabilization [
79]. Furthermore, increasing CaMKII ac-
tivity by knockdown of an endogenous CaMKII inhibitor
protein leads to enhanced memory destabilization [
80].
Rossetti et al. [
9] found that after memory formation over-
expression of constitutively active CaMKII impairs mem-
ory and in his review John Lisman writes that these results
would
‘
support the concept that memory is mediated by
an LTP-like process dependent on CaMKII
’
. However,
considering the evidence discussed above, the occlusion/
saturation test for CaMKII is inconclusive; it is very likely
that the memory impairment observed by Rossetti et al.
[
9] is caused by enhanced memory destabilization rather
than a deficit in memory access.
An intriguing alternative to the saturation/occlusion
test is an enhancement test. The idea behind such test is
that overexpression of the memory-storing protein after
memory formation (including consolidation) would lead
to increased memory due to elevated expression at rele-
vant synapses. Yadin Dudai
’
s lab demonstrated the en-
hancement of previously consolidated memory by PKM
ζ
overexpression, supporting the PKM
ζ
hypothesis [
81].
In addition to a saturation/occlusion test, John Lisman
proposed a necessary test as criteria. This test involves
blocking the function of a candidate memory-storing pro-
tein already at the time of training. The necessary test is
most powerful for exclusion of a candidate mechanism
when the blockade does not prevent memory storage.
Both
α
CaMKII and PKC
ζ
/PKM
ζ
knockout mouse lines
can form spatial memory [
50, 54]. Thus, in principle, these
knockout studies reject both the
α
CaMKII and the PKM
ζ
hypothesis. However, it is not known whether long-term
retention of spatial memory is impaired in these knock-
outs. Moreover, both knockout mouse lines have a com-
pensatory upregulation by related kinase isoforms [
50, 54].
Such compensation does not occur in some knockin mu-
tants. In calcium/calmodulin-binding-deficient-
α
CaMKII
(T305D) mutants there is no compensatory translocation
of ßCaMKII into the postsynaptic density and spatial
memory formation is prevented [
54]. Threonine-286
autophosphorylation-deficient
α
CaMKII (T286A) mutants
also cannot form spatial memory, even when environmen-
tal enrichment is provided [
31, 82]. Thus,
α
CaMKII
knockin studies pass the necessary test, unlike the
α
CaM-
KII knockout studies. However, it should be pointed out
that aversive, hippocampus- and amygdala-dependent
memories can be formed in the T286A knockin mutants,
indicating that the threonine-286 autophosphorylation of
α
CaMKII is not required for some types of memory stor-
age [
34, 56, 83, 84]. Therefore, the model provided by Ros-
setti et al. [
9], and discussed in John Lisman
’
s review, may
only apply to spatial memory storage and not storage of
other types of memory.
The key test to determine the necessity of a
memory-storing molecule is the erasure test. The eras-
ure test consists of functional inactivation after memory
has been formed and this inactivation should cease be-
fore memory is tested (to exclude an impact on re-
trieval). Many studies have shown that treatment with
ZIP (PKM
ζ
inhibitory peptide) erases established mem-
ories [
85]. Almost all types of memories seem to be sen-
sitive to ZIP erasure. However, ZIP not only blocks
PKM
ζ
, but also other molecules [
48
–
50]. Thus, currently
there is no erasure test to support the PKM
ζ
hypothesis.
In contrast Rossetti et al. [
9] suggest that they have car-
ried out a successful spatial memory erasure by blocking
CaMKII function. However, in my opinion more experi-
mental evidence is needed to assure that Rossetti et al.
specifically blocked CaMKII function. They overex-
pressed
α
CaMKII with a point mutation (K42 M) that
blocks its catalytic activity. Unfortunately, Rossetti et al.
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 5 of 12
did not perform a biochemical characterization of this
overexpression, addressing, for example a change in
CaMKII activity in the postsynaptic density. Other stud-
ies with heterozygous K42 M knockin mutants (express-
ing 50% wild-type
α
CaMKII and 50% mutated
α
CaMKII) have not found a dominant-negative effect on
CaMKII activity in forebrain [
84]. Moreover, John Lis-
man
’
s lab showed that overexpression of the K42M mu-
tant impairs synaptic transmission in a dominant-
negative fashion in hippocampal slices [
86]. However,
K42M knockin mutants do not show impaired synaptic
transmission in the hippocampus [
84]. Taken together,
this suggests that the K42 M overexpression might lead
to unwanted side effects that might be the cause of the
memory erasure.
In conclusion, John Lisman
’
s review presents very im-
portant conceptual points regarding the molecular basis
of memory storage. It also analytically discusses the
PKM
ζ
hypothesis, but is less critical of the CaMKII hy-
pothesis. In my view, both hypotheses could be correct,
but more experimental evidence is needed to ascertain
this.
Commentary on
“
Criteria for identifying the mo-
lecular basis of the engram (CaMKII, PKM
ζ
)
”
.
Contributed by Ji-il Kim, Pojeong Park and Bong-Kiun
Kaang.
John Lisman was a respected scholar with a great pas-
sion for science. Throughout his life, he made outstand-
ing achievements in various scientific fields. In
particular, he devoted most of his career to answering
the question,
“
What is the molecular basis of memory?
”
He hypothesized that a molecular switch triggered by
recent activities could maintain synaptic potentiation
and memory [
4]. Once the molecular switch is activated,
memory can be maintained through a reverberating
positive feedback loop. Lisman found that Ca2+/calmo-
dulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII) has many
properties that met his hypothesis and followed up on
this during his lifetime to prove that this brain-enriched
complex is the
“
memory molecule.
”
In Lisman
’
s posthumously published paper, we can ap-
preciate his unique and great ideas regarding his original
question,
“
What is the molecular basis of memory?
”
[
10]. In particular, unlike the general perspective of the
engram studied at the circuit, cell, and synapse levels
[
87], he stated that the engram constitutes the molecular
changes by which a memory is stored in the brain. In
addition, he marked a milestone in the analysis of the
molecular engram by presenting appropriate criteria in
his paper [
10].
In the first part of this paper, Lisman described the re-
lationship between memory and long-term potentiation
(LTP) at a glance and discussed why and how studying
LTP contributes to understanding the nature of memory.
In a similar vein, he also emphasized the importance of
understanding the molecular mechanism of LTP main-
tenance to identify the memory molecule.
In addition, Lisman suggested three types of tests (Ne-
cessary, Saturation/Occlusion, and Erasure tests), which
would be used to evaluate the role of molecules in the
maintenance of LTP and memory. By comparing two
major candidates of the memory molecule, protein kin-
ase M
ζ
(PKM
ζ
) and CaMKII, based on his three types
of tests, he suggested that CaMKII, rather than PKM
ζ
,is
more appropriate as the memory molecule.
Subsequently, Lisman divided the LTP process into
three different phases: Induction (the triggering of signal
cascades by, for example, calcium), Maintenance (the
throwing of a molecular switch such as autophosphoryl-
ation of CaMKII) and Expression (involving the down-
stream targets (AMPA receptors) of the activated
Maintenance molecule(s)). Memory, Lisman argues, will
only be permanently erased by manoeuvres that attack
the molecular mechanisms responsible for Maintenance.
This subdivision is of importance to understand whether
a candidate molecule is specifically responsible for LTP
maintenance, rather than for other phases. Therefore,
Lisman emphasized that before a candidate molecule is
interpreted as being important in the maintenance
process, it should be confirmed whether the impairment
of LTP is caused by a perturbation during the induction
or expression processes. Data interpretation based on
these subprocesses might reduce any confusion in
searching for the memory molecule regulating the main-
tenance process, which underlies storage of the engram.
In summary, Lisman
’
s insightful theories, such as of the
three types of tests for evaluating candidate molecules
and interpretation based on subprocesses, will serve as
significant milestones for future memory research.
Despite Lisman
’
s significant contributions, some im-
portant questions remain that future generations should
pursue to fully understand the molecular basis of
memory.
1. It is clear that synaptic potentiation occurs after ap-
propriate stimuli, and the synaptic potentiation between
engram cells underlies memory storage [
88]. Considering
the relationship between synaptic potentiation and mem-
ory, Lisman insisted that maintenance of synaptic potenti-
ation by the molecular switch (that is, CaMKII) is the
mechanism that maintains memory over a long period of
time. In other words, the enhancement of the synapses in
which the potentiation occurs during learning should be
maintained persistently to store the memory.
However, the place where memory is stored in our
brain might be highly dynamic. It is well known that epi-
sodic memories in rodents are initially stored in the
hippocampus for several weeks, but are gradually trans-
ferred to cortical areas [
89]. Moreover, recent studies
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 6 of 12
have demonstrated that engram cells in various brain re-
gions are continuously changing [
90
–
93]. According to
these results, synapses that store memory are seemingly
not a static, but a highly dynamic structure after learn-
ing. If the engram cells are dynamically changing, then
the memory-storing engram synapses might also be dy-
namically changing, and the enhancement of synapses
that are initially activated during learning may not need
to be sustained for a long time to maintain memories.
Therefore, although it is necessary to study how molecu-
lar cascades could be maintained over a long period of
time at the synapse level, future studies will need to
adopt a systems approach to explain how a qualitatively
similar memory can be preserved in the light of memory
engram dynamics.
2. In addition to CaMKII, many other molecules such
as PKM
ζ
and cytoplasmic polyadenylation element-
binding protein (CPEB) are known to be involved in
memory maintenance [
38, 94]. As Lisman insisted, CaM-
KII might be the only molecule that meets the three cri-
teria, but there are considerable data that other
molecules are also involved in memory maintenance. In
particular, PKM
ζ
is of interest as a key molecule in
memory maintenance because it could be constitutively
active with the lack of a regulatory subunit [
38, 50].
Some evidence suggests that PKM
ζ
is crucial to maintain
synaptic potentiation not only in memory, but also in
chronic pain [
95] and drug addiction [
96]. In addition,
PKM
ζ
is also involved in epigenetic mechanisms, which
might be related to long-term regulation of gene expres-
sion for memory maintenance [
66]. Therefore, it is ne-
cessary to reconcile these data with an integrated view.
Interestingly, memory can also be abolished when a
constitutively active form of CaMKII is overexpressed
[
9], whereas an enhanced memory has been observed
following PKM
ζ
overexpression [
81]. Considering the
role of CaMKII, which is also important in memory in-
duction, this memory deficit could be a result of indis-
criminate spine maturation by excessive CaMKII
activity, which might destroy patterns that encode the
memory [
9]. In contrast, PKM
ζ
, which is known to be
insufficient to induce spine maturation, may be activated
by another mechanism (such as phosphoinositide-
dependent protein kinase-1) to maintain memory [
97].
Thus, PKM
ζ
might not be able to cause indiscriminate
spine maturation just by its overexpression, but it could
possibly induce over-maturation by functioning to a
greater extent in engram spines where maturation has
already occurred. Therefore, the molecular cascade in-
cluding CaMKII and PKM
ζ
should be further investi-
gated to better understand the detailed molecular
mechanisms of memory maintenance.
John Lisman
’
s contributions to science, and especially
his contributions to the molecular basis of memory were
original, wide-ranging and important. Future generations
must answer the remaining questions to better under-
stand the biological foundations of memory.
Commentary on
“
Criteria for identifying the mo-
lecular basis of the engram (CaMKII, PKM
ζ
)
”
.
Contributed by Mary B. Kennedy.
John Lisman was a devoted and serious neuroscientist
who pursued his chosen problems and ideas with pas-
sion. Among these was a question which he formulated
as: what mechanisms underlie the formation of
“
the en-
gram?
”
The engram is an abstraction that means slightly
different things to different people. Some consider it to
be the more or less permanent neural circuits that
underlie specific episodic memories. To John, it was
“
the
molecular changes by which a memory is stored in the
brain.
”
Of course, one can easily assume that the mo-
lecular changes he refers to are those that form and
maintain the more or less permanent neural circuits that
underlie specific episodic memories.
My own career has been devoted to understanding
what I refer to as the biochemical mechanisms that
regulate synaptic strength in excitatory synapses and
thus allow synapses to adapt their signaling patterns to
environmental input, and to store memories. The slight
differences in emphasis between my conception of syn-
aptic regulation and John
’
s statements about
“
the en-
gram
”
go a long way toward explaining why he and I so
often disagreed profoundly about the interpretation of
various experiments and about the experimental way
forward to understand synaptic regulation and memory.
When I was an assistant professor, I and my students
set out to use the protein chemistry methods that I still
revere, to discover and understand the function of regu-
latory proteins in the postsynaptic density. My first stu-
dents and I began by purifying a calmodulin-dependent
protein kinase (later named CaMKII) that, as a postdoc
in the Greengard laboratory, I had first measured in
brain homogenates and identified as located in a particu-
lar fraction of proteins eluted from a DEAE-cellulose
column. We quickly realized that it was highly abundant
in brain, and that it appeared to be a quantitatively
significant component of the postsynaptic density
[
98
–
100]. While studying the enzymatic properties of
the purified protein, my student, Steve Miller, noticed
that the kinase activity seemed to retain some phos-
phorylating activity in the absence of calcium and cal-
modulin after it had phosphorylated itself in the
presence of these two activators. During a visit to my
lab, John pointed out that this behavior might fit a
theoretical model he had proposed regarding a way for
an activated state to outlast turnover of individual
molecules and we mentioned this in our paper [
4, 20].
The notion of a calcium-dependent switch excited
many neuroscientists and John began assuring me that
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 7 of 12
we might have discovered
“
the memory molecule.
”
This
notion, and the reaction of some neuroscientists to it,
began to disturb me. My conception of regulatory bio-
chemistry did not and does not include the notion that a
single molecule would constitute
“
memory.
”
I had, and
have, great respect for the complexity of molecular evo-
lution. I believe that subcellular
“
states,
”
especially
powerful synaptic connections that are important for
brain function, must be generated and maintained by in-
tricately controlled and robust mechanisms. Even the
pathways of bacterial metabolism are filled with feed-
back regulation and alternative pathways. How much
more complexity must have evolved to accurately regu-
late a process as subtle and important as mammalian
episodic memory and insure its robustness? I resisted
the idea that CaMKII was
“
the memory molecule
”
for
this reason. There is no
“
memory molecule.
”
Synapses
are regulated by the mutual interactions of a whole net-
work of proteins. It was clear then and is even clearer
now, that CaMKII is a critical component of synaptic
regulation because it is abundant in the brain and post-
synaptic density, it responds to calcium coming through
NMDA receptors, and retains its activation for a short
period that is delimited by the opposing activity of pro-
tein phosphatases. We now know that CaMKII feeds
back to AMPA and NMDA receptors by phosphorylat-
ing them, and it can initiate changes in many down-
stream regulatory enzymes including nitric oxide
synthase, synGAP, and regulators of the actin cytoskel-
eton. But it is also true that calcium and calmodulin in
the spine directly regulate a large number of other pro-
cesses, including several that depend on
cAMP-dependent protein kinases, and various forms of
protein kinase C. Thus, I have always believed that an
important way forward to understand synaptic regula-
tion is to study in vitro the mutual interactions among
the regulatory molecules in the postsynaptic density and
spine, test whether and when those interactions are im-
portant for synaptic function, and learn the quantitative
kinetic parameters that govern these interactions so that
we can gradually account completely for observed func-
tional changes by modeling the dynamic action of bio-
chemical regulatory loops and pathways (for a beginning
effort see [
101]).
John took a very different approach. His earliest train-
ing was in physics, and his primary technical expertise
was in electrophysiology. He worked by using logic to
create abstractions like
“
the engram
”
,or
“
induction, ex-
pression, and maintenance;
”
then trying to devise ways
that he and others could use pharmacology, electro-
physiology and sometimes genetics to deduce molecular
mechanisms. To my mind, these abstractions involve
myriad assumptions that are often unfounded and tend
to obscure the subtleties of actual molecular
mechanisms. Each of these
“
phases
”
of LTP that can be
measured with an electrode encompasses many under-
lying molecular mechanisms. I think that a defining dis-
agreement between John and me, and indeed between
me and a host of synaptic electrophysiologists, is that
one cannot tease out and understand the molecular
mechanisms of synaptic plasticity with electrodes,
pharmacology, and genetics alone. Each of these
methods is useful, but without an understanding of the
subtleties of biochemical interactions of proteins in a
regulatory network, the picture will always be incom-
plete, and may often be badly oversimplified. John was
surely not alone in his conviction that he could reason
through fundamental molecular mechanisms of learning
with logic and electrophysiological experiments in intact
tissues and animals. This thinking still permeates the
NIH review panels at every level and has damaged fund-
ing of in vitro biochemical studies. In turn, this situation
has damaged the ability of basic neuroscience to provide
the knowledge that pharmaceutical companies need to
identify possible therapeutics for Alzheimer
’
s disease,
mental illnesses, and autism spectrum disorders.
It is evident that both CaMKII and PKM
ζ
play crucial
roles in the mechanisms of synaptic plasticity and in
maintaining synaptic strength. There is no reason to see
their importance as mutually exclusive. A more useful
way forward will be to search out the crucial
“
substrate
proteins
”
that these two fascinating enzymes regulate. It
is even possible that CaMKII plays an important struc-
tural role in the postsynapse. In vitro biochemical work
will be needed to fully understand the interlocking func-
tions of the synaptic regulatory networks.
I am inclined to agree with a comment by Richard
Morris [
102] in a discussion of a recent paper from the
Sacktor lab on PKM
ζ
[50]. Morris reminds us that a
“
molecule implicated in memory retention really does
(not) need to be sustained throughout the lifetime of a
memory. An alternative possibility is that it may trigger
structural changes that are, in turn, mediated by other
molecules (such as actin): thus, with this job done, our
memory molecule can gracefully depart the scene to play
upon another stage. Such structural changes could then
be faithfully recycled during routine protein turnover,
with these proteins being unaware, so to speak, that they
are sustaining a memory.
”
When a synapse is made
more powerful by the addition of more AMPA recep-
tors, more active zones, a larger spine and postsynaptic
density, etc., the collective energy embodied in the myr-
iad protein affinities imparts great structural stability.
Removal or dislodging of any one or two or three pro-
teins will not disrupt the structural stability imparted by
all the other associations. Thus, normal cellular repair
and maintenance processes could continue to maintain
the larger synapse once it is established. These cellular
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 8 of 12
processes are not perfect; yet we all know that memory
isn
’
t perfect either and memories are often lost or al-
tered as we age.
John Lisman made many contributions to neurosci-
ence. His passion and his provocative comments and
ideas will be missed.
The non-erasable memory of John Lisman.
Contributed by Richard G M Morris.
John Lisman was a man who was infectiously curious,
read voraciously, thought deeply and was genuinely in-
terested in what others had to say. He was a listener. He
was both passionate about science and generous to
others around him who were pursuing their scientific in-
terests; indeed the recent appreciations of him by friends
and colleagues [
103 ] are very moving. He was a man
with diverse scientific interests ranging from phototrans-
duction, the possibility that autophosphorylation of
CaMKII could be a molecular switch for memory, neural
oscillations and working memory storage and, later in
his career, the puzzle of schizophrenia. This diversity led
him into contact with all manner of different people in
neuroscience that don
’
t often connect. John Lisman was
a man who loved science and for whom science was -
beyond family - his life.
In his Paths to Discovery contribution to the 3rd edi-
tion of the Bear, Connors and Paradiso textbook of
neuroscience [
104], John describes how he came up with
the idea of an autophophorylation as a molecular switch
for memory. Inevitably, it was during a walk along a
beach - the best ideas often come near the sea:
“
My eureka moment
...
. was that a group of
autophosphorylating kinase molecules localized at a
synapse could make a stable switch. During LTP
induction, these molecules would become
phosphorylated, and this would make them active. If a
kinase molecule was dephosphorylated or replaced in
the course of protein turnover, it could be
phosphorylated by other members of the group. The
switch could stay on, perhaps indefinitely, and this
showed how unstable molecules could produce stable
information storage.
”
In this final paper on which we are commenting [
10],
John asserts that the definitive evidence that CaMKII au-
tophosphorylation could be a mechanism of memory
comes not from correlational data showing that this
process occurs in a lasting manner in response to neural
activity that may be associated with memory formation
in synaptic or behavioural models - for such data (im-
portant as they are) are merely suggestive of a memory
storage mechanism. Rather, it is from a particular type
of experiment from which a causal inference can be
drawn. This is his
“
erasure
”
test. The argument is
attractively simple: if CaMKII autophosphorylation is a
mechanism of memory storage, memory must disappear
if you turn it off.
The argument has depth too for, in Figure 1, he de-
fines the critical distinction between an induction
process, a maintenance process and an expression
process with respect to the making of a lasting memory
trace, and at several points in his article asserts (cor-
rectly in my view) that evidence from expression experi-
ments is not necessarily relevant to induction or
maintenance processes. We must keep these distinctions
separate. However, while not in any way demurring from
this argument, I am not convinced it is sufficient - for a
more
‘
top-down
’
perspective on memory would distin-
guish the dissociable processes of memory-encoding,
storage, consolidation and retrieval. Re-activation and
retrieval did not seem to be on John
’
s radar and this
omission from his thinking may be critical. From a psy-
chological perspective, the supposition is that memory
traces in long-term memory can be active or can be-
come dormant (i.e.
‘
inactive
’
) over weeks, months or
even years. And then - magically as it were - be reacti-
vated and retrieved. Does the autophosphorylation idea
require that this biochemical process is going on dis-
creetly at dormant synapse after dormant synapse across
these time-periods? Here I have a certain suspension of
disbelief for a pre- and post-synaptic structural solution
at the synapse feels (to me) much more economical. A
metaphor may be helpful here to get across the general
idea: Consider the task of a supermarket store-manager
trying to maximise his or her sales. They need to devote
sufficient length and space in an aisle to the sale of dif-
ferent items, and not allow an item that doesn
’
t sell very
well to occupy too much space. Given this - the range of
(say) coffee items might take up a metre or two, but
should not occupy more. The memory in the system
here is the length of the shelf devoted to coffee, itself a
reflection of manager
’
s experience of how quickly they
are taken off the shelves by the shop
’
s customers. But
the items - the coffee jars - are temporary and unstable.
In this way, stability and instability co-habit happily,
even though there is an analogy to the protein turnover
that rightly concerned John in his thinking about CaM-
KII. His ideas about
“
slots
”
in the PSD was very much in
the same spirit.
But this need not mean Lisman
’
s autophosphorylation
idea is wrong. To the contrary, if we build into his
framework the important distinction between cellular
(i.e. immediate) and systems consolidation (longer-term),
it seems entirely possible that sustained CaMKII auto-
phosphorylation during the initial cellular phase may be
critical for memory as he thought. What is important
about this division into two qualititatively distinct con-
solidation systems, from the perspective of evolution, is
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 9 of 12
that a cellular consolidation process takes things
‘
off--
line
’
from the perspective of cell firing (the relevant
brain cells can get on with other new learning). How-
ever, this biochemical process serves, for a while, as an
on-line
‘
long-list
’
of what is destined to be retained in-
definitely in the brain
’
s lasting long-term memory sys-
tems. That is, post-processing, a body of information is
being retained for potentially quite long periods (hours,
certainly; days, maybe) even though not all of it may be
retained by the longer-term, and probably structural,
systems consolidation process.
The data presented in Figure 4 and 5 show Lisman
’
s
favoured
‘
saturation
’
and
‘
erasure
’
tests for establishing a
role for CaMKII autophosphorylation in memory storage.
In the former test, animals learn to avoid a specific place
in Andre Fenton
’
s ingenious place avoidance task (see
path data for Trial 12). At that point, an HSV vector ex-
pressing activated CaMKII is infused into the hippocam-
pus with the expected mechanistic consequence being a
disruption of the spatial pattern of synaptic weights. Mem-
ory is then poorer (but not in the control condition) on
Trial 13 (6 days later). This is more of a memory retrieval
test than any other, and I note that the inspiration for this
experiment was Vegard Brun
’
s similar study in which LTP
itself was saturated [
105 ]. The
‘
erasure
’
test is different. In
this case again, a memory is first acquired - either in a
model system such as LTP or in behaviour. The idea then
is to erase this memory using TatCN21 or HSV-K42 M
which interferes with the ability of CaMKII to bind effect-
ively with the NMDA receptor. When given after 4 days of
training, loss of memory was demonstrated in a test con-
ducted 10 days later after, apparently, the dominant nega-
tive construct was no longer present. This means that the
loss of memory is not a transient consequence of it not
being expressed well in the presence of the interfering
agent. The memory trace is really gone. But the encoding
system is not damaged and re-learning can occur. By any
standards, this is a powerful experimental design from
which strong inferences can indeed be drawn.
But where does this leave us? I share John
’
s assertion
that
“
the molecular basis of memory storage is one of
the most fundamental questions in cellular neurosci-
ence
”
. (page 9). I am less sure that we are yet in a situ-
ation where we can all go home and say
“
problem
solved
”
. Part of my reticence is that I feel uncertain
about any theory that, implicitly or explicitly, implies
that a specific molecule is the be-all and end-all of
everything. Even in the case of DNA, which comes
pretty close to meeting that sense of
“
problem solved
”
with respect to the issue of replication, there were still
all manner of issues that have sensibly occupied molecu-
lar biologists for years since. With respect to neurosci-
ence, I am not sure we are yet in a position to think that
the autophosphorylation of CaMKII is quite in the DNA
league, but also doubt that we can think of the problem
of memory in merely a cellular way. I have already noted
what feels to me to be the weakness of a theory that ap-
parently requires a biochemical autophosphorylation
process to be continuing indefinitely.
Beyond these comments, there are larger issues to do
with why some memories are remembered and not others.
My own reflections on this issue relate to
synaptic-tagging-and-capture [
106
–
108 ] and to how an
initial cellular consolidation process that provides the first
level of information
“
selection
”
interfaces with a systems
consolidation process that serves to update existing know-
ledge structures such as schemas [
109 , 110 ]. One joy of
my career was discussing these matters with John Lisman
by email, SKYPE and in person, especially in relation to
his theory with Tony Grace about the impact that novelty
might have on selective retention within the hippocampus
mediated via the neuromodulatory transmitter dopamine
[
111 ]. My laboratory was inspired by this idea and con-
ducted a number of experiments on the issue, some of
which went on to influence John
’
s own thinking. More re-
cently, our suggestion that he, and Julie Frey earlier were
right about dopamine but might not have been right about
the source of the dopamine, was as intriguing to him as to
ourselves. He made a point of attending our poster pre-
sentations at the Society for Neuroscience (2015) showing
behavioural, electrophysiological and optogenetic data
pointing to the locus coeruleus rather than the ventral teg-
mental area as possible cells of origin. These data were
eventually published a year later [
112 ], followed shortly by
data from Eric Kandel
’
s lab [
113 ] indicating the presence
of dopamine release from activated LC axons. The jury is
still out on this issue, but the suggestion is at least out
there. Adrian Duszkiewicz and Tomonori Takeuchi both
got a buzz from having John go through their posters so
constructively with them - and I listened with interest. To
the end, John cared about young scientists. Let us never
erase our memory of such a generous, graceful and gifted
scientist.
Authors
’
contribution
All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher
’
sNote
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Received: 3 August 2018 Accepted: 24 October 2018
References
1. Lisman JE. Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable
synapses reliable. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20(1):38
–
43.
2. Lisman JE, Idiart MA. Storage of 7 +/
−
2 short-term memories in oscillatory
subcycles. Science. 1995;267(5203):1512
–
5.
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 10 of 12
3. Otmakhova N, Duzel E, Deutch AY, Lisman J. The hippocampal-VTA loop:
the role of novelty in controlling the entry of information into long-term
memory. In: Intrinsically Motivated Learning in Natural and Artifical
Systems; 2012. p. 235
–
54.
4. Lisman LE. A mechanism for memory storage insensitive to molecular
turnover: a bistable autophosphorylating kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
1985;82(9):3055
–
7.
5. Lisman LE, Goldring MA. Feasibility of long-term storage of graded
information by the Ca
2+
/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase molecules of
the postsynaptic density. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1988;85(14):5320
–
4.
6. Lisman J. The CaM kinase II hypothesis for the storage of synaptic memory.
Trends Neurosci. 1994;17(10):406
–
12.
7. Lisman J, Schulman H, Cline H. The molecular basis of CaMKII function in
synaptic and behavioral memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):175
–
90.
8. Lisman J, Yasuda R, Raghavachari S. Mechanisms of CaMKII action in long-
term potentiation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(3):169
–
82.
9. Rossetti T, Banerjee S, Kim C, Leubner M, Lamar C, Gupta P, Lee B, Neve R,
Lisman J. Memory erasure experiments indicate a critical role of CaMKII in
memory storage. Neuron. 2017;96(1):207
–
16.
10. Lisman J. Criteria for identifying the molecular basis of the engram (CaMKII,
PKM
ζ
). Mol Brain. 2017;10(1):55.
11. Lisman J, Goldring M. Evaluation of a model of long-term memory based
on the properties of the Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase. J
Physiol Paris. 1988;83(3):187
–
97.
12. Lisman J. A mechanism for the Hebb and the anti-Hebb processes
underlying learning and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1989;86(23):
9574
–
8.
13. Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New
York: Wiley; 1949.
14. Bliss TV, Gardner-Medwin AR. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic
transmission in the dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following
stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232(2):357
–
74.
15. Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the
dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the
perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232(2):331
–
56.
16. Andersen P, et al. Specific long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission
in hippocampal slices. Nature. 1977;266(5604):736
–
7.
17. Abraham WC. How long will long-term potentiation last? Philos Trans R Soc
Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2003;358(1432):735
–
44.
18. Bradshaw KD, Emptage NJ, Bliss TV. A role for dendritic protein synthesis in
hippocampal late LTP. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18(11):3150
–
2.
19. Crick F. Memory and molecular turnover. Nature. 1984;312(5990):101.
20. Miller SG, Kennedy MB. Regulation of brain type II Ca2+/calmodulin-
dependent protein kinase by autophosphorylation: a Ca2+
−
triggered
molecular switch. Cell. 1986;44(6):861
–
70.
21. Lee HK, et al. Regulation of distinct AMPA receptor phosphorylation sites
during bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Nature. 2000;405(6789):955
–
9.
22. Tomita S, et al. Bidirectional synaptic plasticity regulated by phosphorylation
of stargazin-like TARPs. Neuron. 2005;45(2):269
–
77.
23. Dunwiddie TV, Lynch G. The relationship between extracellular calcium
concentrations and the induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation.
Brain Res. 1979;169(1):103
–
10.
24. Collingridge GL, Kehl SJ, McLennan H. Excitatory amino acids in synaptic
transmission in the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway of the rat
hippocampus. J Physiol. 1983;334:33
–
46.
25. Malenka RC, et al. Postsynaptic calcium is sufficient for potentiation of
hippocampal synaptic transmission. Science. 1988;242(4875):81
–
4.
26. Dudek SM, Bear MF. Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of
hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;89(10):4363
–
7.
27. Mulkey RM, Malenka RC. Mechanisms underlying induction of
homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of the hippocampus.
Neuron. 1992;9(5):967
–
75.
28. Mulkey RM, Herron CE, Malenka RC. An essential role for protein
phosphatases in hippocampal long-term depression. Science. 1993;
261(5124):1051
–
5.
29. Mulkey RM, et al. Involvement of a calcineurin/inhibitor-1 phosphatase cascade
in hippocampal long-term depression. Nature. 1994;369(6480):486
–
8.
30. Zeng H, et al. Forebrain-specific calcineurin knockout selectively impairs
bidirectional synaptic plasticity and working/episodic-like memory. Cell.
2001;107(5):617
–
29.
31. Giese KP, et al. Autophosphorylation at Thr286 of the alpha calcium-
calmodulin kinase II in LTP and learning. Science. 1998;279(5352):870
–
3.
32. Malinow R, Schulman H, Tsien RW. Inhibition of postsynaptic PKC or CaMKII
blocks induction but not expression of LTP. Science. 1989;245(4920):862
–
6.
33. Buard I, et al. CaMKII
“
autonomy
”
is required for initiating but not for
maintaining neuronal long-term information storage. J Neurosci. 2010;
30(24):8214
–
20.
34. Murakoshi H, et al. Kinetics of endogenous CaMKII required for synaptic
plasticity revealed by Optogenetic kinase inhibitor. Neuron. 2017;94(3):690.
35. Lengyel I, et al. Autonomous activity of CaMKII is only transiently increased
following the induction of long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus.
Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20(11):3063
–
72.
36. Lee SJ, et al. Activation of CaMKII in single dendritic spines during long-
term potentiation. Nature. 2009;458(7236):299
–
304.
37. Chang JY, et al. CaMKII autophosphorylation is necessary for optimal
integration of ca(2+) signals during LTP induction, but not maintenance.
Neuron. 2017;94(4):800
–
8 e4.
38. Sacktor TC. How does PKM
ζ
maintain long-term memory? Nat Rev Neurosci.
2011;12(1):9
–
15.
39. Westmark PR, et al. Pin1 and PKM
ζ
sequentially control dendritic protein
synthesis. Sci Signal. 2010;3(112):ra18.
40. Kelly MT, Crary JF, Sacktor TC. Regulation of protein kinase Mzeta synthesis by
multiple kinases in long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 2007;27(13):3439
–
44.
41. Yao Y, et al. PKM zeta maintains late long-term potentiation by N-
ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor/GluR2-dependent trafficking of postsynaptic
AMPA receptors. J Neurosci. 2008;28(31):7820
–
7.
42. Ling DS, Benardo LS, Sacktor TC. Protein kinase Mzeta enhances excitatory
synaptic transmission by increasing the number of active postsynaptic
AMPA receptors. Hippocampus. 2006;16(5):443
–
52.
43. Schuman EM. Synapse specificity and long-term information storage.
Neuron. 1997;18(3):339
–
42.
44. Steward O. mRNA localization in neurons: a multipurpose mechanism?
Neuron. 1997;18(1):9
–
12.
45. Pastalkova E, et al. Storage of spatial information by the maintenance
mechanism of LTP. Science. 2006;313(5790):1141
–
4.
46. Serrano P, et al. PKM
ζ
maintains spatial, instrumental, and classically
conditioned long-term memories. PLoS Biol. 2008;6(12):2698
–
706.
47. Hsieh C, et al. Persistent increased PKM
ζ
in long-term and remote spatial
memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017;138:135
–
44.
48. Lee AM, et al. Prkcz null mice show normal learning and memory. Nature.
2013;493(7432):416
–
9.
49. Volk LJ, et al. PKM-zeta is not required for hippocampal synaptic plasticity,
learning and memory. Nature. 2013;493(7432):420
–
3.
50. Tsokas P, Hsieh C, Yao Y, Lesburgueres E, Wallace EJC, Tcherepanov A,
Jothianandan D, Hartley BR, Pan L, Rivard B et al. Compensation for PKMzeta
in long-term potentiation and spatial long-term memory in mutant mice.
Elife 2016;5:e14846.
51. Hinds HL, Tonegawa S, Malinow R. CA1 long-term potentiation is
diminished but present in hippocampal slices from alpha-CaMKII mutant
mice. Learn Mem. 1998;5(4
–
5):344
–
54.
52. Silva AJ, et al. Deficient hippocampal long-term potentiation in alpha-
calcium-calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. Science. 1992;257(5067):201
–
6.
53. Silva AJ, et al. Impaired spatial learning in alpha-calcium-calmodulin kinase II
mutant mice. Science. 1992;257(5067):206
–
11.
54. Elgersma Y, et al. Inhibitory autophosphorylation of CaMKII controls PSD
association, plasticity, and learning. Neuron. 2002;36(3):493
–
505.
55. Cooke SF, et al. Autophosphorylation of alphaCaMKII is not a general
requirement for NMDA receptor-dependent LTP in the adult mouse. J
Physiol. 2006;574(Pt 3):805
–
18.
56. Irvine EE, Vernon J, Giese KP. AlphaCaMKII autophosphorylation
contributes to rapid learning but is not necessary for memory. Nat
Neurosci. 2005;8(4):411
–
2.
57. Lledo PM, et al. Calcium/calmodulin-dependent kinase II and long-term
potentiation enhance synaptic transmission by the same mechanism. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92(24):11175
–
9.
58. Schuette SR, et al. Overexpression of protein kinase Mzeta in the
Hippocampus enhances long-term potentiation and long-term contextual
but not cued fear memory in rats. J Neurosci. 2016;36(15):4313
–
24.
59. Bejar R, et al. Transgenic calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II activation:
dose-dependent effects on synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory. J
Neurosci. 2002;22(13):5719
–
26.
Bear
et al. Molecular Brain
(2018) 11:76
Page 11 of 12