of 24
1
1
Supplementary Information
for
2
Experience
-
dependent plasticity in an innate social behavior is mediated by
3
hypothalamic LTP
4
Stefanos Stagkourakis
1
*
, Giada Spigolon
2
, Grace Liu
1
, David
J. Anderson
1,3
*
5
6
*
Corresponding author information:
Stefanos Stagkourakis, David J. Anderson
7
Email:
stefanos.stagkourakis@caltech.edu
,
wuwei@caltech.edu
8
9
10
11
This PDF file includes:
12
Supplementary Information text
13
Figures S1 to S5 with legends
14
T
able
S1
15
SI References
16
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.2011782117
2
Supplementary Information Text
17
Extended
D
iscussion
18
The plasticity observed at AHiPM
VMHvl
Esr1
synapses likely has both post
-
and pre
-
synaptic
19
components, as suggested by an increase in the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio
(1
-
3)
following aggression
20
training (
Fig. 2J, K), and by the differential responses of VMHvl
Esr1
neurons to trains of pre
-
synaptic stimuli
21
(4, 5)
(Fig. 2L, M), respectively.
Surprisingly,
the
form
of
hypothalamic
LTP
studied
here
does
not
exhibit
22
“occlusion,”
phenomenon
observed
in
studies
of
hippocampal
or
amygdalar
LTP
(6,
7)
,
in
which
following
23
in
vivo
behavioral
induction
of
LTP
in
the
synaptic
population
of
interest,
the
magnitude
of
LTP
that
can
24
be
induced
subsequently
ex
vivo
is
markedly
decreased
(Fig.
4D,
K).
Similarly,
we
do
not
observe
the
25
related
phenomenon
in
which
prior
in
vivo
LTP
can
enhance
the
extent
of
LTD
that
can
be
induced
ex
26
vivo
in
slices
from
such
animals.
The
reason(s)
for
the
failure
to
observe
these
phenom
ena
are
not
clear,
27
and
will
require
further
studies
to
elucidate.
There
are
a
number
of
effects,
however,
which
could
account
28
for
these
observations.
Firstly,
it is possible that the proportion of synapses modified by the
in vivo
social
29
experience was smal
l compared to the synapses being sampled in the slice. Another possibility is that
30
the synapses being assayed in the slice are a different population than the ones modified
in vivo
, or lastly
31
that new synapses were formed by the
in vivo
experience and they
are the ones primarily contributing to
32
the LTP and LTD being measured
in vitro
. This last possibility is of particular interest, given that
-
as
33
presented in Fig. 3, an increase in spine density occurs in VMHvl
Esr1
neurons of AGG mice.
34
The data on LTP pre
sented here, blur the distinction between neural circuits mediating learned
35
vs. innate behaviors, and reinforce the concept of “learned innate behavior,” in which plasticity within
36
developmentally hardwired circuits can function to modify the strength of a
n instinctive behavior in
37
response to social experience
(8, 9)
. An example of the latter in an invertebrate is the post
-
mating
38
response in
Drosophila,
a form of memory in which female sexual receptivity is inhibited following mating
39
(10
-
12)
.
40
3
This idea notwithstanding, more complex forms of learning, such as classical or operant
41
conditioning, may utilize circuits that are parallel to those that mediate innate forms of the modified
42
behavior, as shown in the case of condi
tioned
vs
.
innate
fear
(13
-
15)
. In this context, it is worth noting
43
that mice can learn an instrumental, operant response using successful aggressive encoun
ters as a
44
reinforcer
(16)
, and that performance
of this instrumental task is facilitated by optogenetic activation of
45
VMHvl neurons
(17)
. The neural substrates and synaptic mechanisms underlying this operant
46
conditioning remain to be elucidated, although the nucleus accumbens
-
based reward system has been
47
implicated in recent studies
(18)
.
48
Aggressiveness can be enhanced not only by repeated successful agonistic enco
unters, as
49
shown here, but also by prior mating experience
(19)
. Recently, we showed that as little as 30 minutes
50
of free social interaction with a female was sufficient to transfor
m a socially naive mouse into an AGG
51
mouse within 24 hrs of the interaction
(8)
. This effect was associated with a change in the neural
52
representation of ma
le
vs
. female conspecifics among VMHvl
Esr1
neurons, from partially overlapping to
53
largely non
-
overlapping
(8)
. W
hether this change in neural population coding involves synaptic plasticity
54
within VMHvl, or is inherited from upstream structures, such as the MeA
(9)
, remains to be determined.
55
In other studies, we have shown that the effect of social isolation stress to promote aggression in non
-
56
sexually experienced males is mediated by the neuropeptide Neurokinin B (NkB) and its receptor Nk3R,
57
acting in the
dorso
-
medial hypothalamus (DMH)
(20)
. The relationship of this form of experience
-
58
dependent plasticity to VMHvl
Esr1
neuronal activity is currently unknown.
59
Our current findings also provide insights into individual differences in the ability of genetically
60
identical animals to re
spond to “aggression training”.
Firstly,
we
show
here
that
several
physiological
61
parameters
in
AGG
mice
are
different
from
those
in
socially
naïve
mice.
These
include
elevated
baseline
62
VMHvl
Esr1
neuron
activity
(Fig.
1G
-
J),
increased
spontaneous
excitatory
input
onto
VMHvl
Esr1
neurons
63
(Fig.
2A
-
C),
increased
AMPA/NMDA
ratio
at AHiPM
VMHvl
Esr1
synapses
(Fig.
2I
-
K)
and
altered
synaptic
64
integration
properties
(Fig.
L,
M).
By
contrast,
in
NON
mice
th
e
spontaneous
inhibitory
inputs
to
VMHvl
Esr1
65
4
neurons
are
increased,
relative
to
socially
naïve
mice
(Fig.
S1).
In
addition,
NON
mice
exhibit
shorter
66
lasting
LTP
and
longer
lasting
LTD
than
are
observed
in
AGG
mice
(Fig.
4D,
G).
Whether
increased
LTD
67
is
suf
ficient
to
account
for
the
failure
of
NON
mice
to
respond
to
aggression
training
is
not
yet
clear.
68
Another
possibility,
suggested
by
the
increased
spontaneous
IPSCs,
is
that
VMHvl
receives
stronger
69
inhibitory
input
from
GABAergic
neurons
in
NON
mice.
While
there
are
very
few
GABAergic
neurons
70
within
VMHvl
itself
(21)
,
VMHvl
receives
strong
inhibitory
input
from
the
neighboring
tuberal
(TU)
region.
71
It
is
possible
that
the
lack
of
aggression
in
NON
mice
reflects
potentiation
of
these
TU
GABAergic
72
neurons.
The
synaptic
mechanisms
responsible
for
the
lack
of
aggression
in
NON
mice
will
clearly
require
73
further
investigation.
74
We also find th
at NON mice have low
er
levels of circulating T in comparison to AGG mice, and
75
that
experimental administration of supplemental T can restore the capacity for “aggression learning” in
76
such animals. While the role of T in promoting male aggressiveness is wel
l
-
established
(22
-
27)
, our
77
studies provide new insight into the neurophysiological mechanisms that may mediate this effect in the
78
context of aggression training. Sp
ecifically, we observe that NON animals can only express LTP
in vivo
79
following administration of exogenous T. Although LTP can be induced optogenetically
ex vivo
in slices
80
from control NON animals, LTP in slices from T
-
implanted NON animals exhibited higher
-
amplitude and
81
persistence.
Moreover,
in AGG mice
,
levels of T increased during aggression training. This correlation
82
suggests either that T acts directly to en
hance LTP at this synapse, which in turn promotes aggression,
83
or that T acts indirectly, by promoting aggressive behavior which in turn enhances LTP (Fig. S5). Whether
84
T directly influences synaptic plasticity, and if so the underlying molecular mechanisms
involved, as well
85
as the mechanistic basis of individual differences in T levels, are interesting topics for future study.
86
Our experiments have focused on a specific glutamatergic input to VMHvl
Esr1
neurons which have
87
a causal role in aggression. In addi
tion to our finding
s
, recent work
reported
that
VMHvl
-
projecting
Vglut
+
88
neurons
in
the
AHiPM
are
active
during
both
social
investigation
and
attack,
and
that
functional
89
manipulation
of
these
neurons
influences
attack
(28,
29)
.
VMHvl
Esr1
neurons receive inputs from neurons
90
5
in over 30 different structures
(30)
, raising the quest
ion of whether other
glutamatergic
inputs to these
91
cells also display plasticity. Indeed, recently published work has identified synaptic plasticity promoted
92
by foot
-
shock stress in a medial amygdala projection that primarily targets the central part of VM
H (VMHc)
93
(31)
. Although a causal role in promoting aggression was not directly demonstrated for this input, and the
94
mechanism of potentiation was not established, plasticity at this synapse may regulate stress
-
induced
95
aggression
(31)
. The present study demonstrates that AHiPM
VMHvl
Esr1
synapses can undergo
96
Hebbian LTP, and that potentiation of these synapses occurs during social experience that enhances
97
offensive aggression. Together, these data suggest that VM
Hvl likely provides a substrate in which
98
aggression plasticity can occur at multiple synaptic inputs, each of which may play distinct roles in
99
physiology and/or behavior. Our results also reveal striking effects of aggression training on dendritic
100
spine mo
rphology among VMHvl
Esr1
neurons, although we cannot be certain whether the secondary
101
dendritic branches where we observe this phenomenon receive synaptic input from AHiPM. Other recent
102
studies have identified structural plasticity among VMHvl
PR
-
derived ax
ons innervating hypothalamic
103
targets in females, which are mediated by changes in sex steroids during estrus
(32)
. The present work,
104
together with these other studies, begins to provide a view of the acute and dynamic changes that can
105
occur through experience and/or hormonal modulation,
in a brain node that controls innate social
106
behaviors.
107
108
109
Extended M
aterials and
M
ethods
110
Animals.
All mice were housed in ventilated micro
-
isolator cages in a temperature
-
controlled
111
environment (median temperature 23 °C), under a reversed 12h dark
-
light cycle, and had
ad libitum
112
access to food and water. Mouse cages were changed weekly on a fixed day o
n which experiments were
113
not performed.
114
6
Brain slice electrophysiology.
Acute mouse brain slices were prepared. Slices were cut on a vibratome
115
(Leica VT1000S) to 300
μ
m thickness and continuously perfused with oxygenated aCSF containing (in
116
millimolar): NaC
l (127), KCl (2.0), NaH
2
PO
4
(1.2), NaHCO
3
(26), MgCl
2
(1.3), CaCl
2
(2.4), and D
-
glucose
117
(10). See also
SI Appendix,
Table S1. Whole
-
cell current
-
and voltage
-
clamp recordings were performed
118
with micropipettes filled with intracellular solution containing (in millimolar), K
-
gluconate (140), KCl (10),
119
HEPES (10), EGTA (10), and Na
2
ATP (2) or Cesium methanesulfonate (140)
, KCl (10), HEPES (10),
120
EGTA (10), and Na
2
ATP (2) (pH 7.3 with KOH). Recordings were performed using a Multiclamp 700B
121
amplifier, a DigiData 1440 digitizer, and pClamp 11 software (Molecular Devices). Slow and fast
122
capacitative components were semi
-
automat
ically compensated. Access resistance was monitored
123
throughout the experiments, and neurons in which the series resistance exceeded 15 M
Ω
or changed
124
≥20% were excluded from the statistics. The liquid junction potential was 9.7 mV and not compensated.
125
The r
ecorded current was sampled at 20 kHz. Baseline recordings of EPSCs, IPSCs and optogenetically
-
126
evoked synaptic currents were performed in normal aCSF conditions and in the absence of GABA and
127
NMDA
receptor
blockers. Spontaneous excitatory currents
were sam
pled at the reversal of Cl
-
(V
HOLD
=
-
128
70 mV), and spontaneous inhibitory currents were sampled at the reversal of fast excitatory
129
neurotransmission (V
HOLD
=0 mV). All recordings were performed at near
-
physiological temperature
130
(33±1
o
C). Reagents used in slice
electrophysiology experiments; Neurobiotin
TM
tracer (Vector
131
laboratories) was used in combination with Streptavidin conjugated to Alexa Fluor 647. MATLAB and
132
OriginPro9 were used for electrophysiological data analysis.
CNQX (10
μ
M), D
-
AP5 (25
μ
M), TTX (50
0
133
nM), and 4
-
AP (100 mM) were bath applied to block excitatory transmission and to test if optogenetically
134
evoked responses are monosynaptic
(28)
. All drugs were pre
-
applied for 5 min in the slice chamber prior
135
to data acquisition.
136
Brain slice Ca
2+
i
maging.
The spontaneous activity of mouse VMHvl
Esr1
neurons was monitored by
137
imaging fluorescence changes of the jGCaMP7s biosensor, using a CCD camera (Evolve
®
512,
138
Photometrics), mounted on an Olympus BX51WI
microscope. Recordings were 5 min in duration.
As a
139
7
subpopulation of VMHvl
Esr1
neurons
likely
expresses T
-
type Ca
2+
channels, the Ca
2+
transients reported
140
in Fig. 1
reflect
both action
potentials
and subthreshold synaptic potentials. A 60x water
-
dipping objective
141
was used to focus on VMHvl. Ca
2+
imaging analysis was performed using the MIN1PIPE one
-
photon
142
based calcium imaging signal extraction pipeline
(30)
, in combination with custom
-
written MATLAB
143
routines.
144
Cell filling and reconstruction.
Mouse
Esr1
+
VMHvl neurons were recorded in whole
-
cell mode with
145
intracellular pipette solution as above, with the addition of 0.2% neurobiotin. After recording, slices were
146
placed in fixative (4% paraformaldehyde/0.16% picric acid), washed in PBS and incubated at 4°C
for 72h
147
in a solution containing s
treptavidin conjugated to Alexa Fluor 647
. After extensive washing, slices were
148
mounted with 2.5% DABCO in glycerol. VMHvl
Esr1
neuron identity of all filled cells was confirmed with
149
colocalization studies of viral
-
induced
tdTomato expression.
150
Ex vivo
optogenetics.
Photostimulation during slice whole
-
cell recordings was performed via a 3.4 watt
151
535 nm LED mounted on the microscope fluorescence light source and delivered through the 60X
152
objective’s lens. Photostimulation w
as controlled via the analog outputs of a DigiData 1440A, enabling
153
control over the duration and intensity. The photostimulation diameter through the objective lens was
154
~310
μ
m with illumination intensity typically scaled to 0.35 mW/mm
2
.
155
In vivo
optogeneti
cs.
Subjects were coupled via a ferrule patch cord to a ferrule on the head of the
156
mouse using a zirconia split sleeve (Doric Lenses). Ferrules and fiber
-
optic patch cords were purchased
157
from Thorlabs and Doric Lenses, respectively. The optical fiber was c
onnected to THORLABS fiber
-
158
coupled LED (
M530F2, 9.6 mW
) via a fiber
-
optic rotary joint (
FRJ_1x1_FC
-
FC,
Doric Lenses) to avoid
159
twisting of the cable caused by the animal’s movement. Prior to a testing session, following the coupling
160
of the patch cords with
the optic fiber ferrules,
Esr1
Cre/
+
animals were given 10 min to acclimate in their
161
home cage in the absence of an intruder. The frequency and duration of photostimulation were controlled
162
using the programmable train generator Pulse Pal
(31)
. Light power was controlled by dialing an analog
163
8
knob on the LED driver (T
-
Cube
TM
LED Driver with Trigger Mode, Thorlabs, LEDD1B). Light power was
164
measured from the tip of the ferrule in the patch cord at different laser output settings, using an o
ptical
165
power energy meter and a photodiode power sensor (Thorlabs, PM100D, and S130VC). L
ight power was
166
dialed at 0.5 mW at the fiber tip.
Upon identification of the fiber placement coordinates in brain tissue
167
slides, irradiance (light intensity) was calcu
lated using the brain tissue light transmission calculator based
168
on (
http://www.stanford.edu/group/dlab/cgi
-
bin/graph/chart.php
) using laser power measured at the tip
169
and the distan
ce from the tip to the target brain region measured by histology. Animals showing no
170
detectable viral expression in the target region and/or ectopic fiber placement were excluded from the
171
analysis.
172
In vivo
electrophysiology.
In vivo
electrophysiology
recordings were performed in freely moving mice,
173
using chronic silicon probe implants. All extracellular recordings were conducted in the left VMHvl, and
174
all mice included in the present study were validated using the following criteria: identification of
the
175
lipophilic dye (DiD) tract targeting VMHvl, phototagging of VMHvl
Esr1
neurons, and photostimulation
-
176
evoked low
-
latency attack against a conspecific through optrode mediated VMHvl
Esr1
neuron
177
photoactivation. Recordings were performed using an optrode ba
sed on the A1x32
-
Poly2
-
10mm
-
50s
-
178
177 NeuroNexus probe and a 100
μ
m optic fiber placed along the probe’s shank terminating 50
μ
m above
179
the probe’s first recording sites. Photostimulation was delivered using fiber
-
coupled Thorlabs LEDs
180
(M530F2,
9.6 mW
for LTP
/LTD studies, and M617F2
, 13.2 mW
for phototagging), and light power was
181
dialed at 0.33 mW at the optrode’s fiber tip. The probe was implanted 200 μm above the intended
182
recording site, and using the NeuroNexus OH32LP oDrive was lowered over a period of fou
r days to the
183
target coordinates (lowering by 50
μ
m/day).
Only channels that showed photo
-
responses in the local field
184
potential were used for LFP analysis.
Recordings were performed using the Open Ephys acquisition
185
board with a
sampling rate of 30 kHz, th
e Open Ephys I/O board, and the Open Ephys GUI
(31)
. The
186
LFP
signal was obtained by applying low pass
-
filtering with a cut
-
off at 100 Hz on the raw voltage traces.
187
Note that although the exc
itation wavelength for testing LTP
in vivo
was chosen to preferentially activate
188
9
AHiPM synaptic input to VMHvl neurons via Chronos, due to spectral overlap between the opsins we
189
cannot exclude a contribution of direct ChrimsonR
-
mediated Esr1 neuron activat
ion to the fEPSPs
.
190
Immunohistochemistry.
Mice were anesthetized with ketamine (KetaVed, VEDCO) and xylazine
191
(AnaSed, NDC 59399
-
110
-
20), then transcardially
perfused with 20 mL of ice
-
cold fixative. Whole brains
192
were dissected, immersed in ice
-
cold fixative for 90 min then stored in 0.1M PBS (pH 7.4) containing
193
20% sucrose, 0.02% bacitracin and 0.01% sodium azide for three days, before freezing with dry ice.
194
Coronal sections were cut at a thickness of 14 μm on a cryostat (Microm, Walldorf) and thaw
-
mounted
195
onto gelatine
-
coated glass slides. For GFP staining, brain sections were incubated overnight at 4°C using
196
a chicken anti
-
GFP antibody (Aves Labs, Inc., GFP
-
1010) at 1:500 dilution. For tdTomato staining brain
197
sections were incubated overnight at 4°C using a rabbit anti
-
DsRed antibody (Takara, 632392) at 1:500
198
dilution. Primary antibody incubation was followed by Alexa
-
488
-
conjugated goat anti
-
chicken secondar
y
199
antisera (1:500; Invitrogen), and/or Alexa
-
568
-
conjugated donkey anti
-
rabbit secondary antisera (1:500;
200
Invitrogen). DAPI solution (1mg/mL) was used at 1:10000 dilution.
For further details on reagents, see
201
also
SI Appendix
,
Table S1.
202
Confocal microscopy
.
Brain slices were imaged by confocal microscopy (Zeiss, LSM 800). Brain areas
203
were determined according to their anatomy using Paxinos and Franklin Brain Atlas
(8, 33)
.
204
For
cell
reconstructions,
each
entire
neurobiotin
-
filled
neuron
was
acquired
at
63X
(NA
=
1.4),
1
μm
step
205
size
using
a
Zeiss
LSM880
confocal
microscope.
Imaris
9.3
(Bitplane)
was
used
to
visualize
the
topology
206
of
the
dendritic
tree
and
the
centrifugal
branch
ordering
method
was
chosen
to
sort
dendrites,
assigning
207
order
1
to
the
root.
2
nd
order
dendrites
were
then
selected
for
further
imaging
acquisition
to
perform
spine
208
quantification.
70
-
90
μm
-
long
dendritic
segments
were
acquired
at
63X
(NA
=
1.43),
0.1
μm
step
size
209
and
0.06x0.06
pixel
-
size
using
Airy
-
scan
detector
at
the
LSM880.
Two
segments
were
acquired
for
210
dendrites
longer
that
200
μm.
211
10
For
spine
quantification,
images
of
dendritic
segments
were
rendered
in
Imaris
using
the
Blend
algorithm
212
and
the
Filament
module
was
used
to
reconstruct
dendrites
and
spines.
Specifically,
the
auto
-
path
213
method
was
chosen
and
thinnest
spine
diameter
(between
1.5
and
2
μm),
maximal
distance
from
the
214
dendrite
(between
3
and
8
μm)
and
fluorescence
intensity
threshold
were
defined
in
every
single
dendrite
215
to
detect
spines.
The
statistics
module
in
Imaris
was
used
to
extract
spine
density
values.
Three
to
six
216
segments
per
neuron
were
quantified
and
values
were
averaged.
217
Tail
-
tip whole blood sampling.
Whole blood samples of 40
-
70
μ
L were collected from the lateral tail
218
vein of
restrained mice
(32)
. Only blood samples acquired within
2 min post
-
restraining were used for
219
hormone measurements, and the subjects were then returned to their home cage. Briefly, the rodent’s
220
tail was immersed for 30 sec in 40
o
C water to dilate the tail blood vessels. Immediately after, a 23G
221
needle was used
to puncture the lateral tail vein, and whole blood was collected. Bleeding was stopped
222
via applying gentle pressure to the tail at the level of the puncture with surgical cleaning tissue, and 2%
223
chlorhexidine antiseptic solution was used for tail disinfect
ion at the end of the procedure. Blood samples
224
were refrigerated at 4
o
C for 30 min and then centrifuged at 4
ο
C at 2000 RCF. Following centrifugation,
225
serum was collected and was frozen at
-
80
o
C for a maximal period of 2 months prior to performing ELISA
226
mea
surements. All blood samples were acquired during the dark phase of the 12h/12h light/dark cycle.
227
For further details on reagents, see also
SI Appendix
,
Table S1.
228
Testosterone ELISA.
96
-
well plates were used in a ready
-
to
-
use kit for testosterone ELISA (R&
D
229
systems
Catalog number KGE010). Linear regression was used to fit the optical densities for the
230
standard curve
vs
the concentration. The standard curve range for corticosterone was 300 to 100000
231
pg/mL. Concentrations were calculated from the optical de
nsity at 450 nm of each sample. Appropriate
232
sample dilutions were carried out to maintain detection in the linear part of the standard curve and
233
typically involved 1 to 10 for mouse serum samples. Data acquired from the performed ELISAs are
234
presented as ab
solute values. Differences between groups were identified by Student’s
t
-
test or ANOVA.
235
11
Viral vectors.
For
ex vivo
Ca
2+
imaging studies of VMHvl neurons,
Esr1Cre/
+
male mice
were injected
236
in VMHvl with 200 nL of AAV9
-
Syn
-
FLEX
-
jGCaMP7s
-
WPRE (addgene
104491
-
AAV9) 5.3 × 10
12
237
genomic copies per mL. For
ex vivo
optogenetic studies,
Esr1Cre/
+
male mice
were injected in VMHvl
238
with 200 nL of AAV9
-
FLEX
-
tdTomato (addgene 28306
-
AAV9) 4.2 × 10
12
genomic copies per mL and in
239
AHiPM with 100 nL of AAV5
-
Syn
-
Chronos
-
GFP (addgene 59170
-
AAV5) 3.7 × 10
12
genomic copies per
240
mL. For
in vivo
optogenetic and electrophysiology experiments,
Esr1Cre/
+
male mice
were injected in
241
VMHvl with 100 nL of AAV5
-
Syn
-
FLEX
-
rc[ChrimsonR
-
tdTomato] (addgene 62723
-
AAV5) 4.1 × 10
12
242
genomic co
pies per mL and in AHiPM with 100 nL of AAV5
-
Syn
-
Chronos
-
GFP (addgene 59170
-
AAV5)
243
3.7 × 10
12
genomic copies per mL. Control groups were injected in VMHvl with 100 nL of AAV9
-
FLEX
-
244
tdTomato (addgene 28306
-
AAV9) 4.2 × 10
12
genomic copies per mL and in AHiPM w
ith 100 nL of AAV5
-
245
CAG
-
GFP (37825
-
AAV5) 5.9 × 10
12
genomic copies per mL.
For further details on reagents, see also
SI
246
Appendix
,
Table S1.
247
Stereotactic surgery and viral gene transfer.
Adult heterozygous
Esr1Cre/
+
males were single
-
housed
248
for at least five
days before undergoing surgical procedures and were operated on at 16
20 weeks of
249
age.
Mice were anesthetized using isoflurane (5% induction, 1
2% maintenance, in 95% oxygen) and
250
placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments). Body temperature was
maintained using a
251
heating pad. An incision was made to expose the skull for stereotaxic alignment using the inferior cerebral
252
vein and the Bregma as vertical references. We based the coordinates for the craniotomy and stereotaxic
253
injection of VMHvl on an
anatomical magnetic resonance atlas of the mouse brain (AP:
4.68 mm; ML:
254
±0.78 mm; DV:
5.80 mm), as previously described
(34)
. Virus suspension was injected using a pulled
-
255
glass capillary at a slow rate of 8
10 nL/min, 100 nl per injection site (Nanojector II, Drummond Scientific;
256
Micro4 controller, Wor
ld Precision Instruments). The glass capillary was withdrawn 10 min after the
257
cessation of injection.
258
12
Osmotic mini
-
pumps.
Testosterone was dissolved at 30 mg/ml in sesame oil and was administered for
259
2 weeks at a rate of 0.75 mg/hour via subcutaneous osmot
ic mini
-
pumps (Alzet, model 1002)
(35)
.
For
260
further details on reagents, see also
SI Appendix
,
Table S1.
261
Social behavior assays.
The aggressio
n phenotype of animals defined as aggressive (AGG), or non
-
262
aggressive (NON) in the present study was based on the expression of aggressive behavior in the five
263
consecutive day resident
-
intruder test (5cdRI). Animals that did not express any aggressive beha
vior in
264
the 5cdRI were identified as NONs, while all AGGs expressed aggression in a minimum of three out of
265
the five trials, with the majority expressing attack behavior in all five days. As described in Fig. 1, the
266
5cdRI composed of a 15 min social intera
ction test per day in the resident’s home arena, with socially
267
naïve 4
-
5 month
-
old residents. Intruders were BALB/c males 2
-
3 months old and of lower weight/size.
268
Three follow up tests were performed in the 5cdRI experimental design presented in Fig. 1, sp
ecifically,
269
2 weeks, 4 weeks and 12 weeks following
the completion date of the 5cdRI assay. Note that only 15 out
270
of a total of 106 aggressive mice, were used to quantify the effect of aggression training. This was based
271
on the finding that following behav
ioral analysis of the first 15 mice used in the study, the power of the
272
ANOVA test reached
P
< 0.0001. This suggested that including additional observations would not aid the
273
power of the statistical test. In Fig. 5, following the 5cdRI, on day six a social interaction test was
274
performed
in a novel home
-
cage
-
sized arena. In addition to the C57 mal
e, a male with a larger
275
bodyweight/size CD
-
1 conspecific was introduced. The duration of this experiment was 15 min, following
276
which both animals were returned to their home cage.
277
Statistics.
No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes bu
t our sample sizes are
278
similar to those reported in previous publications
(36)
. Data met the assumptions of the stati
stical tests
279
used and were tested for normality and variance.
Normality was determined by D’Agostino
Pearson
280
normality test. All
t
-
tests and one
-
way ANOVAs were performed using GraphPad Prism software
281
(Graphpad Software Inc.). Statistical significance was
set at
P
<
0.05.
282
13
283
284
285
286
287
Fig. S1. Presynaptic plasticity of inhibitory input in VMHvl
Esr1
neurons of non
-
aggressive male
288
mice.
289
(A) Representative recordings of spontaneous inhibitory post
-
synaptic currents (sIPSCs) from VMHvl
Esr1
290
neurons, from socially naive, aggressive (AGG) and non
-
aggressive (NON) mice.
291
(B) Left
cumulative frequency distribution plot of sIPSC inter
-
event interval (IEI) in voltage
-
clamp
292
recordings collected from VMHvl
Esr1
neurons from socially naive, AGG and N
ON mice (n=11
-
14 VMHvl
Esr1
293
neuron recording per group, collected from 8
-
10 mice per group, Kolmogorov
-
Smirnov test,
P
< 0.0001
294
between socially naive and AGG mice,
P
< 0.0001 between socially naive and NON mice). Right
295
comparison of sIPSC frequency in vo
ltage
-
clamp recordings collected from VMHvl
Esr1
neurons from
296
socially naive, AGG and NON mice (n=11
-
14 VMHvl
Esr1
neuron recording per group, collected from 8
-
10
297
mice per group, Kruskal
-
Wallis one
-
way ANOVA with uncorrected Dunn’s post hoc test,
P
= 0.0425
298
between socially naive and AGG mice,
P
= 0.0480 between socially naive and NON mice).
299
(C) Left
cumulative frequency distribution plot of sIPSC amplitude in voltage
-
clamp recordings
300
collected from VMHvl
Esr1
neurons from socially naive, AGG and NON mice (
n=11
-
14 VMHvl
Esr1
neuron
301
recording per group, collected from 8
-
10 mice per group, Kolmogorov
-
Smirnov test,
P =
0.2780 between
302
socially naive and AGG mice,
P
< 0.0001 between socially naive and NON mice). Right
comparison of
303
sIPSC amplitude in voltage
-
cla
mp recordings collected from VMHvl
Esr1
neurons from socially naive, AGG
304
14
and NON mice (n=11
-
14 VMHvl
Esr1
neuron recording per group, collected from 8
-
10 mice per group,
305
Kruskal
-
Wallis one
-
way ANOVA with uncorrected Dunn’s post hoc test,
P
= 0.8995 between socially
306
naive and AGG mice,
P
= 0.0476 between socially naive and NON mice).
307
ns; not significant, *
P
< 0.05, ****
P
< 0.0001.
In box plots the median is represented by the center line,
308
the interquartile range is represented by the box edge
s, the bottom whisker extends to minimal value,
309
and the top whisker extends to the maximal value.
310
15
311
312
313
314
315
Fig. S2. Monosynaptic connectivity between AHiPM and VMHvl
Esr1
neurons.
316
(A) Schematic illustration of the experimental design, transducing AHiPM neuron
s with Chronos and
317
optically evoking postsynaptic responses in VMHvl
Esr1
neurons
ex vivo
.
318
(
B
) Quantification of VMHvl
Esr1
neurons with optically
-
evoked EPSCs (oEPSCs).
319
(C) Averaged amplitudes of oEPSCs evoked on baseline (green), TTX (magenta), TTX + 4AP (
orange),
320
and in CNQX and AP5 (black); n=5 brain slices, collected from n=5 mice, one
-
way ANOVA with Dunnett’s
321
post hoc test,
P
= 0.0002 between baseline and TTX conditions,
P
= 0.0001 between baseline and
322
TTX+4AP conditions,
P
= 0.0002 between baseline and
CNQX+AP5 conditions. Shaded region
323
represents the standard error. The vertical scale bar defines current and the horizontal scale bar time.
324
***
P
< 0.001.
In box plots the median is represented by the center line, the interquartile range is
325
represented by
the box edges, the bottom whisker extends to minimal value, and the top whisker extends
326
to the maximal value.
327
16
328
Fig. S3. Characterization of LTP
-
inducing stimulation protocols at the
AHiPM
VMHvl
Esr1
synapse
.
329
(A) Schematic of the experimental design used to identify the appropriate stimulation protocol for LTP
330
induction
ex vivo
in socially naïve mice.
331
(B) Illustration of the experimental protocols tested to to induce LTP in the AHiPM
VMHvl synapse.
332
(C) Monitori
ng the optically induced EPSC (oEPSC) prior to, and following application of each of three
333
stimulation protocols (n=8 cells, n=5 socially naïve mice).
334
(D) Alternative quantification/illustration of optically induced EPSC (oEPSC) prior to, and following
335
application of each of three stimulation protocols (n=8 cells, n=5 socially naïve mice
similar to panel C).
336
337
17
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
Fig. S4. Optogenetic induction of LTP at AHiPM
VMHvl
Esr1
synapses in socially naïve mice leads
346
to elevated aggression in the first resi
dent
-
intruder test.
347
(A) Schematic indicative of the experimental design used to induced hypothalamic LTP in the
348
AHiPM
VMHvl synapses, via Chronos
-
eYFP expression in AHiPM, and ChrimsonR expression in
349
VMHvl
Esr1
neurons.
350
(B) Schematic of the behavior test de
sign used to identify whether induction of LTP in the AHiPM
VMHvl
351
synapses, influences the innate expression of aggression.
352
(C) Representative behavior raster plots of a control (YFP) and opsin
-
expressing (LTP) mouse, in the
353
resident
-
intruder test against
a novel BALBc conspecific.
354
(D) Quantification of attack duration (n=5
-
6 mice per group, two
-
tailed unpaired
t
-
test,
P
= 0.0046 between
355
YFP and LTP groups).
356
18
(E) Quantification of attack latency (n=5
-
6 mice per group, two
-
tailed unpaired
t
-
test,
P
= 0.0214 b
etween
357
YFP and LTP groups).
358
(F) Quantification of number of attacks per trial (n=5
-
6 mice per group, two
-
tailed unpaired
t
-
test,
P
=
359
0.0235 between YFP and LTP groups).
360
(G) Quantification of close investigation duration (n=5
-
6 mice per group, two
-
tailed unpaired
t
-
test,
P
=
361
0.7106 between YFP and LTP groups).
362
*
P
< 0.05, **
P
< 0.01.
In box plots the median is represented by the center line, the interquartile range is
363
repre
sented by the box edges, the bottom whisker extends to minimal value, and the top whisker extends
364
to the maximal value.
365
19
366
367
368
369
370
371
Fig. S5. Schematic summary.
372
(A) The schematic summarizes the findings from AGG mice, and the suggested links between
373
aggression,
serum testosterone and hypothalamic LTP.
374
(B) Similar to panel (A), but summarizing results from experiments in NON mice
-
this schematic
375
summarizes the identified links among aggression, serum testosterone and hypothalamic LTP. Our
376
results do not distin
guish whether the effect of elevated serum testosterone to increase LTP
in vivo
(Fig.
377
6P
-
T) is direct, or rather indirect via an effect to increase aggressive behavior, which in turn enhances
378
LTP. However, exogenous administration of T to NON mice (in the
absence of any aggressive
379
experience) enhances LTP amplitude and persistence as tested
ex vivo
(Fig. 6K
-
O).
380
20
Materials and Methods
381
Table S1. Reagents and resources.
382
REAGENT or RESOURCE
SOURCE
IDENTIFIER
Antibodies
Rabbit monoclonal anti
-
DsRed
Takara
632392
Anti
-
GFP rabbit serum
Invitrogen
A
-
6455
Chicken polyclonal anti
-
GFP
Aves Labs, Inc.
GFP
-
1010
Donkey anti
-
Mouse IgG
-
Alexa Fluor
488
ThermoFisher
A
-
21202
Donkey anti
-
Rabbit IgG
-
Alexa Fluor
488
ThermoFisher
A
-
21206
Donkey anti
-
Rabbit IgG
-
Alexa Fluor
568
ThermoFisher
A
-
10042
Donkey anti
-
Rabbit IgG
-
Alexa Fluor
647
ThermoFisher
A
-
31573
Goat anti
-
Chicken IgY
-
Alexa Fluor
488
ThermoFisher
A
-
11039
Biotinylated Goat Anti
-
Rabbit IgG Antibody
Vector Laboratories
BA
-
1000
Donkey anti
-
Rabbit IgG
-
Alexa Fluor
568
ThermoFisher
A
-
10042
Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins
Picric acid
Sigma
-
Aldrich
P6744
4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS
Santa Cruz Biotech.
CAS30525
-
89
-
4
Streptavidin conjugated to Alexa Fluor 647
ThermoFisher
CS32357
Neurobiotin tracer
VectorLabs
SP
-
1120
-
50
Sodium chloride
Sigma
-
Aldrich
S9888
Sodium bicarbonate
Sigma
-
Aldrich
S6297
D
-
(+)
-
Glucose
Sigma
-
Aldrich
G7528
Sodium phosphate monobasic dihydrate
Sigma
-
Aldrich
71505
Potassium chloride
Sigma
-
Aldrich
P9333
Magnesium sulfate heptahydrate
Sigma
-
Aldrich
63138
Calcium chloride dihydrate
Sigma
-
Aldrich
C5080
4
-
Aminopyridine
Sigma
-
Aldrich
275875
CNQX disodium salt
TOCRIS
1045
D
-
AP5
TOCRIS
0106